Be
thankful for comprehensives
|
|
|
|
Donald
Hirsch
|
Monday 10th December 2001
|
|
![](./01pisa_files/image008.gif)
|
The latest
school survey is a blow to advocates of selection: mixing up social classes
reduces inequality without harming overall results. By Donald Hirsch
Over the past decade or so, British schools have been transformed. On the one
hand, parents have greater choice, and local councils have lost many of their
powers to direct children to particular schools through the use of catchment
areas. On the other, central government and its agencies have taken greater
control, imposing a national curriculum, tests and inspections. The aim has
been to raise standards. Have the reforms worked?
At first sight, the results of the latest international survey, carried out
last year and published on 4 December, suggest they have. The Organisation for
Economic Co-operation and Development's Programme for International Student
Assessment puts UK 15-year-olds between third and tenth out of 28
industrialised countries on tests of reading, mathematics and science. We used
only to dream of such placements in the international education league.
The improvement may be linked not just to higher standards, but to a new way of
measuring performance. This latest survey looks at how well 15-year-olds are
prepared for life and for future learning, not through curriculum-based tests,
but through written tasks that invite them to apply knowledge and understanding
in the real world. This gives us more helpful results than the rather crude
surveys that were around in the 1980s, which showed mainly that Japanese
students were better than everyone else at calculating simultaneous equations.
Might Brits be good appliers of knowledge, living up to their reputation, along
with other "Anglo-Saxons", of being pragmatists rather than
theorisers? Possibly. Five primarily English-speaking countries - Australia,
Canada, Ireland, New Zealand and the UK - have done particularly well in this
survey, while Germany was below average. And UK students scored highly on
reading tasks that asked them to evaluate and reflect on texts, rather than on
more technical tasks that asked them, say, to retrieve information. Here, they
scored second only to Canadian pupils, and that by a statistically
insignificant amount.
Thus the survey suggests that the incessant drive for "standards"
hasn't quashed all imaginative thought. Today's teachers are given targets in
the same way that Soviet farmers had wheat quotas, and the new survey found UK
pupils more likely than any others to report that teachers pressure them to do
well academically. The pupils, however, are still able to step back and
reflect.
Yet the old Achilles heel of British education - class difference - is still
there. The difference that social background made to pupil performance in this
survey was greater in the UK than in all but four other countries. Britain's
least privileged students managed to do respectably by international standards
- only 13 per cent of UK pupils had poor reading skills, for example, compared
to 18 per cent overall. But it is the middle classes who have really been
pulling our results up. The quarter of UK students whose parents have the best
jobs score higher on reading than their equivalents in any other country.
Free-market enthusiasts imagined that school choice could change all that.
Allow parents to choose schools, they argued, and standards will rise,
particularly for the disadvantaged, who will no longer have to accept inferior
services. It didn't work out like that. School choice, along with league tables
of test and exam results, did put more pressure on schools to perform well, but
the big winners have been the schools with the most advantaged pupil intakes,
not those with the best teachers. Parental choices are often based less on a
school's educational quality than on the social qualities of its pupils. This
makes education different from most commodities: with a few exceptions, such as
holidays at exclusive resorts, we do not mind much who else is purchasing what
we buy.
Nor is it only in class-conscious Britain that parents are concerned about who
else attends their children's school. In New Zealand, schools that have a
surplus of applicants have been able to draw tortuously shaped admission zones
to keep out children who live close by but in less desirable neighbourhoods.
Research there suggests that the chances of being rejected by a popular school
are closely related to your social class and ethnicity. In France, everything
public is officially equal and school choice is not a policy goal. The reality
is that many parents send their children to subsidised private Catholic
schools, and savvy middle- class parents use all sorts of wheezes to get their children
out of undesirable catchment areas. A recent study found that, among a wave of
middle-class gentrifiers who moved into the not-so-posh Paris suburb of
Nanterre, parents were willing to go public only if others like them did so,
leading to complex "I will if you will" types of social bargaining.
Parents who care about the homes other children come from are often accused of
snobbery. But this latest survey shows that choice of school by social intake
may be more rational than some commentators (including this one) have argued.
It shows that there is only one factor more powerful than a pupil's social
background as a predictor of his or her reading performance at age 15. And that
is the average social background of the other pupils in the school. In other
words, parents can improve their children's academic prospects by choosing a
middle-class school.
But this does not help the system as a whole to improve, because not everyone
can go to a socially above-average school. If school choice allows the children
of pushy middle-class parents to cluster even more than they would otherwise,
educational outcomes will become more polarised. Researchers in the UK cannot
agree on whether choice has indeed caused more social concentration.
Yet we tend in Britain to overstate the importance of which school you go to.
The new survey shows that, on average, only 20 per cent of the variation in
pupil performance is attributable to differences between schools, against 60
per cent in Germany, which has retained a grammar-school-type system.
So be thankful for comprehensives. The new survey shows conclusively that you
can level up and not just down. The predominantly comprehensive schools in the
UK produce better overall results than mainly segregated Germany, while Finland,
which has almost no differences between schools and some of the lowest social
inequalities, has easily the best reading results of any country. The quarter
of Finnish 15-year-olds whose parents have the lowest-status jobs have literacy
levels as high as the average UK pupil. Though our education ministers and
teachers can pat themselves on the back, there is still room for improvement.
Donald Hirsch is an expert on international education trends who is
currently reviewing school choice policies