In worldwide
tests, 15-year-olds in the UK are now stuck in the middle rankings.
Donald Hirsch says we can learn a lot from the results of the latest
international survey.
When worldwide league tables of reading
performance among 15-year- olds were first published in 2001, the
United States and Norway were both exactly average for countries in
the Organisation for Economic Co- operation and Development. Each
country's education minister made a speech.
"As Americans, we don't think it's
good enough to be average," stormed the US minister, thumping the
table and making one of his country's perennial pledges to become
"best in the world".
His Norwegian counterpart calmly told
the press, "Well, we are about middle, and we will look carefully
at the results of other countries to see if we can improve."
As with football, we in the UK find it
hard to accept we might be middling in international education
comparisons. But now that the latest Programme for International
Student Assessment (Pisa) survey shows that this is the case - in
maths and science as well as reading - it is time to start taking a
measured interest in what we could learn from other countries.
Here are four key areas where
international comparisons could help put our own school system's
development in context.
First - different approaches to the
curriculum count for a lot. In some countries, pupils do relatively
well in tasks requiring content knowledge and familiarity with
operations, while in others they excel at thinking about and applying
information in a wider context.
For example, in the 2006 Pisa science
test, pupils in countries such as the Czech Republic and Hungary were
strong when it came to mastering knowledge to explain scientific
phenomena, but much weaker at interpreting and using scientific
evidence.
It is easy to criticise such systems as
unimaginative, and say that science is about understanding, not about
learning facts or theories. In fact, it is about both.
In France, an emphasis on scientific
reasoning produced above-average results in using evidence, but weak
knowledge of scientific phenomena. This was especially the case on
earth and space topics, where pupils performed abysmally, below every
OECD country except Mexico and Turkey.
In the UK, pupils are just above
average both in "knowledge of science" and "knowledge
about science". The French experience shows the importance of
maintaining a balanced approach to improving both areas if we are to
make progress.
The second area in which international
comparisons are helpful is accountability and performance monitoring.
Of the many aspects of school policy examined in the survey,
accountability was one of the few that had a clear association with
performance - even after taking into account other factors such as
pupils' social background. In countries where standards-based external
examinations exist and schools posted their results, pupils tended to
do better.
British schools have been given a much
larger dose of accountability and performance monitoring than most
countries in recent years - and the Pisa results reveal that this has
made a difference. This does not mean, however, that the volume of
testing, inspection and monitoring in the UK is all beneficial. But
countries where the reverse is true tend to become complacent.
This explains Germany's shock at
finding it had below-average standards in the first Pisa test in 2000.
Since then, it has raised its performance to average in reading and
mathematics and above average in science. An important factor in this
appears to be a greater sense of accountability, which helps to keep
teachers and pupils on their toes.
Third - Pisa shows clearly that
selective education produces results that strongly reflect social
differences. The strength of the relationship between social
background and pupil performance varies a great deal across countries:
about a quarter of it is associated with selection. The earlier the
age of selection, the stronger this effect.
This is relevant both to attempts by
some Conservative politicians to put grammar schools back on the
agenda, and even more so to a bitter debate raging in Northern Ireland
over secondary selection.
Abolitionists may take heart from
Poland, which in 1999 created a comprehensive lower secondary sector,
thus delaying selection until 16. The ensuing years saw a huge
improvement in reading performance at 15 among the least able pupils.
The percentage with low proficiency went from well above to well below
the OECD average. And this was not at the expense of strong
performers, whose average scores initially remained static but are now
rising.
Fourth - the performance and character
of school systems can change, sometimes quite quickly. In addition to
the Polish turnaround, one outstanding example is Korea.
In 2000, Korean pupils performed above
average in the Pisa reading test, with very few having low literacy
although few youngsters truly excelled: only 6 per cent of pupils
reached the highest proficiency level, compared with nine per cent
across the OECD.
Korea addressed this shortcoming with a
new curriculum that introduced more essay-based tests and gave pupils
greater scope to present their opinions when being screened for
university. This encouraged better- performing pupils to enhance their
reading and reasoning skills.
The resulting turnaround was truly
remarkable: by 2006, 22 per cent of 15- year-olds - far more than in
any other country - were performing at the top proficiency level.
The kind of rapid change seen in Poland
and Korea in recent years is the exception, and it would be a mistake
to search for a magic bullet to produce comparable improvements in the
UK. Yet the big lesson of international comparison is that we need not
regard the weaknesses of our education system as immutable - even
where they have deep roots in our culture.
As in previous surveys, Pisa identifies
the UK's most important weakness as being its relatively wide gap
between the scores of pupils from different social backgrounds. In the
strongest-performing OECD countries - Finland, Korea and Canada -
these social differences are well below average.
The good news is, if we are truly
determined to follow these countries' examples, we can.
- The writer is an independent policy consultant to
the OECD.
HOW WE COULD CLIMB BACK TO THE TOP OF THE WORLD
- We need to understand that knowledge and how to
apply that knowledge are of equal importance. A balanced approach to
improving both areas of learning is needed.
- There is a strong link between accountability and
performance: too much accountability, in the form of testing and
inspection, can be counterproductive; but too little breeds
complacency.
- Selective schooling widens social gaps - one of
the greatest weaknesses of the UK system. Delaying selection has been
shown to improve literacy among students of all abilities.
- It is possible to effect a fast turnaround in
literacy levels. Where there's a will, there's a way.
Science under the microscope
Donald Hirsch
Published: 07 December 2007
Countries around the world have
been fretting about whether a limited home-grown pool of
scientific know-how will become a brake on growth in
technology-driven economies. In the US, 38 per cent of science
and engineering jobs at doctorate level were filled by
foreign-born students in 2000, up from 24 per cent a decade
earlier. And in the European Union, 700,000 additional
researchers will be required to reach the EU's Lisbon goals in
education and training by 2010.
The most recent 2006 Pisa
assessments looked not just at how well pupils perform in
science, but also at their attitudes and involvement. The good
news is that nearly two-thirds of UK 15-year-olds spend at
least four hours a week in science lessons, twice the
international average and more than any other OECD country bar
New Zealand.
Science is also more infused
throughout the curriculum. For example, environmental topics
are more likely to feature in geography. Perhaps as a result,
Britain's 15-year-olds rank fourth highest in their
environmental awareness.
But other Pisa evidence
suggests that British pupils' interest in science is limited.
After answering questions on, say, acid rain, they were less
likely than pupils in most other countries to say they would
be interested in pursuing these issues.
Even though boys and girls
performed very similarly in the test, their attitudes varied
considerably. Boys had much greater confidence. When asked
about their ability to tackle certain tasks, British and US
boys had more self-confidence than any other nationality, even
though their performance was only 8th and 23rd respectively
among 30 OECD countries.
British girls are near the
bottom in estimation that they are "good at
science". Despite being behind boys in questions focused
on knowledge and explanations, they were ahead on tasks
requiring more general scientific understanding.
In addition, girls enjoy
science less than boys - more so in the UK than most other
countries. The gender gap was also wider than average on
desire to go on studying the subject.
If schools could just get girls
to feel more positive about science, we would be one step
closer to boosting our home-grown pool of future scientists.
Photographs: Chris Thomond.
|
|